Journal of Sports Science and Medicine
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine
ISSN: 1303 - 2968   
Ios-APP Journal of Sports Science and Medicine
Androit-APP Journal of Sports Science and Medicine
Views
9726
Download
953
from September 2014
 
©Journal of Sports Science and Medicine (2014) 13, 439 - 443

Research article
Correct, Fake and Absent Pre-Information Does Not Affect the Occurrence and Magnitude of the Bilateral Force Deficit
Lars Donath1,3, , Tobias Siebert2,4, Oliver Faude1, Christian Puta3  
Author Information
1 Institute of Exercise and Health Sciences, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
2 Institute of Sport Sciences, Department of Motion Science, University of Jena, Jena, Germany
3 Institute of Sport Sciences, Department of Sports Medicine and Health Promotion, University of Jena, Jena, Germany
4 University of Stuttgart, Stuttgart, Germany

Lars Donath
✉ Institute of Exercise and Health Science, Birsstrasse 320B, 4052 Basel, Switzerland
Email: lars.donath@unibas.ch
Publish Date
Received: 09-09-2013
Accepted: 13-02-2014
Published (online): 01-05-2014
 
ABSTRACT

The present study examined whether different pre-information conditions could lead to a volitional modulation of the occurrence and magnitude of the bilateral force deficit (BFD) during isometric leg press. Twenty trained male adults (age: 24.5 ± 1.7 years; weight: 77.5 ± 7.1 kg; height: 1.81 ± 0.05 m) were examined on three days within a week. Isometric leg press was performed on a negatively inclined leg press slide. Each participant completed three maximal isometric strength test sessions with different pre-information conditions given in a graphical chart: no pre-information (NPI; first day), false pre-information (FPI; bilateral force > sum of unilateral forces; second or third day) and correct pre-information (CPI; bilateral force < sum of unilateral forces; second or third day) during bilateral, unilateral-left and unilateral-right leg-press. The sum of left- and right-sided force values were calculated for bilateral (FBL = FBL_left + FBL_right) and unilateral (FUL = FUL_left + FUL_right) analyses. Force data for NPI revealed: Mean (SD): FUL_NPI = 3023 N (435) vs. FBL_NPI = 2812 (453); FPI showed FUL_FPI = 3013 N (459) vs. FBL_FPI = 2843 (446) and the CPI revealed FUL_CPI = 3035 (425) vs. FBL_CPI = 2844 (385). The three (no, false, correct) x 2 (FUL, FBL) rANOVA revealed a high significant main effect of Force (F = 61.82, p < 0.001). No significant main effect of the factor Condition and no significant interaction between Force x Condition was observed. The BFD does not rely on the trueness of the given pre-information (no, false, correct). Cognition-based volitional influences on the BFD on supra-spinal level seem negligible.

Key words: Bilateral force deficit, strength training, lower extremities, unilateral strength


           Key Points
  • BFD is reliable occurring phenomenon
  • Available theoretical knowledge does not affect the BFD
  • Alternating sport should include alternating strength exercises

INTRODUCTION

In a pioneering study Henry and Smith (1961) found that the total force generated in a maximal bilateral hand grip test was significantly reduced compared to the sum of the right and left maximal unilateral hand grip strength. In the last decades force differences between one- and two limb exercises have been reported for various maximal strength tasks (isometric elbow flexion and extension, isometric knee extension, leg press and vertical jumps) (Oda and Moritani, 1995; Koh et al.,1993; Schantz et al., 1989). This phenomenon is usually called bilateral force deficit (BFD) and defined as a reduction in the amount of force produced from bilateral movements of homonymous limbs compared to the sum of forces produced by the left and right limbs when acting alone (Sale, 1992). Depending on the exercise mode (static vs. dynamic, isolated vs. complex tasks), the extent of the BFD was reported to range between 7 to 25 % (Owings and Grabiner, 1998, Jakobi and Chilibeck, 2001). These varying magnitudes of the BFD are considered to be caused by methodological restrictions (randomization and testing procedures), sport-specific stroke patterns (bilateral or unilateral limb movements), training-induced changes of muscle length, muscle activation, muscle fiber types, the type of exercises (multi joint vs. single joint, dynamic vs. static) and motivational prerequisites (Howard and Enoka, 1991).

However, the underlying central as well as peripheral neuromuscular mechanisms leading to lower bilateral strength are still not fully understood. It has been proven that decreased activation of fast motor units (Koh et al., 1993; Secher et al., 1978), reciprocal inhibitions on spinal level and inter-hemisphere inhibition on supra-spinal level (Ohtsuki, 1994; Taniguchi, 1998) may account for the BFD. Interestingly, also volitional modulations have been hypothesized (Koh et al., 1993; Secher et al., 1988).

In this regard, Jakobi and Chilibeck (2001) as well as Secher et al. (Secher et al., 1988) discussed whether a priori achieved information on the theoretical basis of the BFD may influence its occurrence and magnitude. From a physiological point of view, expectations of the force output according to previously achieved information were reported to influence maximal voluntary force and force development (Sahaly et al., 2001). In this regard, sensorimotor feed-forwards related to the expected force-output have been frequently discussed (Blakemore et al., 1998; Diedrichsen et al., 2007). Thus, internal force prediction during strength tasks might rely on the provided visual information, afferent information and expectations. In summary, motor commands appear to be interfered with knowledge and imagery (Lorenzo et al., 2003).

As a consequence, the present study was conducted to examine a sample of non-specifically trained young adults in order to differentiate whether an absent, false and correct pre-information prior to force testing potentially affects the occurrence and magnitude of the BFD during a combined maximal isometric hip- and leg-extension (leg press). Based on the assumption that visual afferent information may provoke expectations of force outputs, we hypothesized that an inverse (false) instruction of the theoretical basis of the BFD might lower the extent of the BFD compared to experiments performed with correct pre-information. From a practical relevance view-point, a verification of this assumption would emphasize (a) the importance of correct instructions due to expected force-outputs (b) to consider the manipulative impact of acute instruction-based placebo/nocebo effects during strength testings.

METHODS

Subjects

Twenty trained young male adults (age: 24.5 ± 1.7 years; weight: 77.5 ± 7.1 kg; height: 1.81 ± 0.05 m, practiced sports disciplines: soccer, n=8; boxing, n=3; powerlifting, volleyball, basketball, swimming, tennis, gymnastics, track and field, each with n = 1; all-round sport, n = 2) were initially examined on a leg press slide (Figure 1) (Ertelt and Blickhan, 2009). None of the participants reported any medication intake and internal as well as orthopedic health impairments (hypertension, knee or hip injuries) that could affect maximal isometric strength testing. The local ethical committee of the University of Jena previously approved this strength measuring approach. No additional approval was needed concerning the present study. Furthermore, the study complied with the declaration of Helsinki and all participants signed an informed written consent prior to the start of the study.

Study design

The present study was conducted as a semi-randomized controlled cross-over trial. All participants were examined on three days within one week on Monday, Wednesday and Friday. Testing was intra-individually performed at the same time of day. On the first testing day, no theoretical pre-information (NPI) concerning the theory of the BFD was provided prior to maximal isometric strength testing. On the second day, a standardized false (incorrect: “you are able to produce higher force values during bilateral leg press, compared to the sum of left- and right-sided unilaterally generated forces”) pre-information (FPI) or the correct pre-information (CPI) was randomly given (“The sum of left- and right-sided unilaterally generated forces is higher than force values during bilateral leg press”). This information was provided with a graphical chart. To ensure that the participants understood the given information, they were asked to repeat and explain the introduced pre-information condition. For each testing day, the order of the tested strength tasks (unilateral left, unilateral right, bilateral) were inter-individually assigned in a random order. The intra-individual testing order was not changed within the three testing days.

Data acquisition and analysis

Randomized isometric strength testing was unilaterally (ULleft, ULright) and bilaterally (BL) performed on each of the three testing days. Three maximal isometric attempts per testing condition (ULleft, ULright, and BL) were captured. Thus, a total amount of nine maximal strength testing attempts was performed on each testing day. The strength testing device (Ertelt and Blickhan, 2009) consisted of two separately placed strain gauge-basing force plates (DMS Type: J2A-06-31k-350, Tetra, Ilmenau, Germany) in order to measure horizontal and vertical (x- and z- components) ground reaction forces up to 30 kN with a sample rate of 500Hz. Data acquisition and operation was controlled by µ-MUSICS“ (Integrated Measurements & Control (IMC), Madison, USA). The x- and z-components were calculated using a software tool (Famos, Integrated Measurements & Control (IMC), Madison, USA) for the total resulting force for each leg. Participants were fixed at the seat using a tension belt and the knee angle was fixed at 90°. The angle was verified by an analogous goniometer. In order to minimize an unintended contribution of the calf muscles, particularly the double-joint gastrocnemius muscle, all participants were requested to press with the plantar heel (Wagner et al. 2006, Siebert et al. 2007) (Figure 1). After a short general warm-up (hopping, stepping and squats within three minutes) and preparing for strength testing, the participants were instructed to press as strong as possible against the heel-block for a total of 3 seconds. Thereby, the participants were constantly encouraged by the vocal instruction “press, press, press” within the strength testing periods.

The consecutively highest force values of the three attempts for each pre-information condition were included into further analysis. The force plate raw data (ascii-files) were processed with Matlab (R14, MathWorksTM, Natrick, MA, USA). Thereby, the sum of left and right sides force values was calculated for bilateral (FBL = FBL_left + FBL_right) and unilateral (FUL = FUL_left + FUL_right) analyses. Force data were provided in Newton. According to the specified equation, the Bilateral Force Index (BIF) was calculated according to Howard and Enoka (1991). Thereby, a negative BIF-value is indicating a BFD.

Statistical analysis

Data were normally distributed (Kolmogorov-Smirnov test) and, thus, presented as means and standard deviations. Then, a 2 Force (BL, ULsum) x 3 Condition (NPI, FPI, CPI) repeated measures analyses of variance (rANOVA) was calculated. Due to the cross-over design both factors were included into analysis as dependent variables. Tukey HSD post hoc tests were conducted in case of a condition effect or Force by Condition interaction.

RESULTS

A BFD was observed in more than 90% of all force measures (Figure 2). The mean BIF-value for NPI was -7.2 ± 4.1%, compared to -5.6 ± 4.8% for FPI and -6.2 ± 3.9 for CPI.

The rANOVA revealed a significant main effect of the factor Force (F = 61.82, p < 0.001). No significant main effect of the factor Condition and no significant interaction between Force and Condition were observed. According to univariate ANOVA computings, means and standard deviations for the bi- and unilateral force values for all pre-information conditions were as follows:

  • no pre- information (NPI): summed unilateral = 3023 ± 435 N vs. bilateral = 2812 ± 453 N (F = 65.8, p < 0.001)
  • false pre-information (FPI): summed unilateral = 3013 ± 459 N vs. bilateral = 2843±446 N (F = 24.7, p < 0.001)
  • correct pre-information (CPI): summed unilateral = 3035 ± 425 N vs. bilateral = 2844 ± 385 N (F = 49.2, p < 0.001).
  • DISCUSSION

    The present study demonstrated for the first time that occurrence and magnitude of the BFD both seem to not depend on the given pre-information condition (no, false, correct) achieved prior to strength testing during a standardized combined hip- and leg extension (leg press) in young adults. Leg-press exercises are considered to reliably reproduce a BFD during isometric exercises (Jakobi and Chilibeck, 2001; Taniguchi, 1998). Independent of pre-information conditions, the mean BIF values ranged between -5.6 and -7.2%. Thus, the magnitude of the BIF is in line with previous findings using isometric leg-press approaches (for review see Jakobi and Chilibeck, 2001).

    In addition to our findings, Koh and coworkers (1993) reported that correct pre-information did not influence the occurrence and magnitude of the BFD. Thus, available correct background information does not interfere with the physiological mechanisms of the BFD. Interestingly, Secher and colleagues (Secher et al., 1988) found an influence of incorrect pre-information on the BFD during leg press. In this study, initially higher BFD values around -20% were observed in a heterogeneous sample of trained and untrained adults. After completing these initial measures, the included participants were incorrectly briefed that the bilaterally produced force output should have been higher than the sum of unilaterally generated force output (inverse BFD explanation). As a consequence, the primarily observed BFD disappeared 5 weeks after the initial measures. Within 5 weeks, all participants had voluntarily access to the training and testing devices in order to familiarize. This procedure was unfortunately not standardized and controlled and the training-induced changes might have affected the BFD. Nevertheless, the bilaterally generated forces did not change between both measures compared to the notably lower unilateral force values. Thus, Secher and coworkers (Secher et al., 1988) assumed that these results might be caused by a volitional reduction of unilateral force production in order to meet the previously incorrectly given explanation of the BFD. In total, the applied study design of Secher and coworkers (1988) might not appraise this issue with certainty. Regarding the hypothesized underlying inhibitory neurophysiologic mechanisms, it would be of more interest if an incorrect pre-information (bilateral higher than unilateral) lead to comparatively higher bilaterally produced force values during post testing compared to pre-testing. Taking these considerations into account together with our findings, it is not likely that the assumed central and spinal neuro-physiological inhibitory mechanisms during bilateral leg-press might be altered by volitional and pre-informational (educational) modulations.

    Since the BFD has been reliably observed in all conditions for nearly all subjects, it appears conceptionally reasonable to consider unilateral-alternating exercises especially in sports disciplines with alternating stroke patterns (e.g. running, swimming, cycling). Thus, conventional strength training using e.g. leg and bench press, squats, push- and chin-ups and dips (especially during specific training periods) are questionable in terms of sports-discipline specific motor patterns of strength requirements. It can be speculated, that this approach might be additionally useful in order to avoid overload-induced injuries during bilateral strength exercises and to enhance neuro-muscular properties by (alternating) unilaterally performed exercises with lower total weights on the spine or trunk. As these conclusions remain speculative and cannot be directly drawn from the present data with certainty, well-designed randomized-controlled intervention studies are needed to evaluate the effectiveness of unilateral vs. bilateral training on sport specific performance. Thereby, it appears not necessary to provide adequate background information on the theoretical basis of the BFD.

    Nevertheless, some limitations of the present study need to be addressed. To apply all relevant pre-information conditions, we could have merely conducted a semi-randomized controlled study design. Although the strength tasks were randomly assigned, the days with pre-information had to be conducted after the initial test with NPI. We additionally did not measure EMG activity or assessed interpolated twitch techniques to achieve further insights into the phenomena of the BFD (e.g evaluating fatigue and potential declines of neuro-muscular activation). Such approaches might give more insights into the neuro-muscular time-course and basis of the BFD.

    CONCLUSION

    In line with previous studies, we can conclude that small but worthwhile BFDs have been repeatedly observed for almost all included participants in all condition. The availability of different pre-information does not relevantly influence the occurrence and magnitude of the BFD during isometric combined leg- and hip extension in young and trained adults. Thus, the BFD does not rely on the trueness of the given pre-information. Knowledge- or cognition-based volitional influences on the BFD on supra-spinal level seem to be negligible.

    From a practical relevance view-point, visually supported instructions given prior to strength testing does not provide relevant potential to manipulate sensory-motor force expectations. However, further longitudinal research on the BFD would be beneficial in terms of addressing EMG-activity adaptation comparing both training approaches (bilateral vs. unilateral-alternation).

    ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

    We appreciate efforts of the participants and the engagement of Carolyn Siebert who prepared and conducted the experiments. Prof. Dr. Reinhard Blickhan (University of Jena) and Prof. Dr. Holger Gabriel (University of Jena) supported the study and provided the infrastructural requirements. Finally, we would like to thank Prof. Dr. Inge Zijdewind (University of Groeningen) for expert proof reading and her valuable suggestions.

    AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY

    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Lars Donath
    Employment: Post-doc, Depart. of Sport, Exercise and Health, University of Basel, Switzerland
    Degree: PhD
    Research interests: Acute and long-term effects of moderate and intense exercise training on postural, strength, endurance and cerebral capacity across lifespan
    E-mail: lars.donath@unibas.ch
     

    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Tobias Siebert
    Employment: Prof., Depart. of Sport and Motion Science, University of Stuttgart, Germany
    Degree: PhD
    Research interests: Modeling and experimental investigation of neuro-muscular systems as well as their chronic and acute (e.g. induced by training) adaptations.
    E-mail: tobias.siebert@inspo.uni-stuttgart.de
     

    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Oliver Faude
    Employment: Head of research of the section ´Exercise and Movement Science´ at the Department of Sport, Exercise and Health at the University of Basel.
    Degree: PhD
    Research interests: Exercise physiology, neuromuscular performance, football science as well as exercise interventions in seniors and children.
    E-mail: oliver.faude@unibas.ch
     

    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Christian Puta
    Employment: Head of Research, Department of Sports Medicine and Health Promotion, Friedrich Schiller University Jena, Germany
    Degree: PhD
    Research interests: Chronic (back) pain, somatosensory and motor information processing, visual-motor interactions, autonomic and inflammatory responses to pain and exercise
    E-mail: christian.puta@uni-jena.de
     
     
    REFERENCES
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Blakemore S.J., Goodbody S.J., Wolpert D.M. (1998) Predicting the consequences of our own actions: the role of sensorimotor context estimation. The Journal of Neuroscience 18, 7511-7518.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Diedrichsen J., Verstynen T., Hon A., Zhang Y., Ivry R.B. (2007) Illusions of force perception: the role of sensori-motor predictions, visual information, and motor errors. Journal of Neurophysiology 97, 3305-3313.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Ertelt T., Blickhan R. (2009) describing force-patterns: a method for an analytic classification using the example of sledge jumps. Journal of Biomechanics 42, 2616-2619.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Henry F.M., Smith L.E. (1961) Simultaneous vs. separate bilateral muscular contractions in relation to neural overflow theory and neuromotor specificity. Research Quarterly in Exercise and Sports 32, 42-47.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Howard J.D., Enoka R.M. (1991) Maximum bilateral contractions are modified by neurally mediated interlimb effects. Journal of Applied Physiology 70, 306-316.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Jakobi J.M., Chilibeck P.D. (2001) Bilateral and unilateral contractions: possible differences in maximal voluntary force. Canadian Journal of Applied Physiology 26, 12-33.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Koh T.J., Grabiner M.D., Clough C.A. (1993) Bilateral deficit is larger for step than for ramp isometric contractions. Journal of Applied Physiology 74, 1200-1205.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Lorenzo J., Ives J.C., Sforzo G.A. (2003) Knowledge and imagery of contractile mechanisms do not improve muscle strength. Perceptual and Motor Skills 97, 141-146.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Oda S., Moritani T. (1995) Movement-related cortical potentials during handgrip contractions with special reference to force and electromyogram bilateral deficit. european journal of applied physiology and occupational physiology 72, 1-5.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Ohtsuki T., swinen s. p., heurer m., massion j., casaer p. (1994) Interlimb Coordination: Neural, Dynamic And Cognitive Constraints.. Changes in strength, speed and reaction time induced by simultaneous bilateral muscle activity. San Diego. Academic.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Owings T.M., Grabiner M.D. (1998) Fatigue effects on the bilateral deficit are speed dependent. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 30, 1257-1262.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Sahaly R., Vandewalle H., Driss T., Monod H. (2001) Maximal voluntary force and rate of force development in humans--importance of instruction. European Journal of Applied Physiology 85, 345-350.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Sale D.G., Komi P.V. (1992) Strength and power in sport. Neural adaption to strength training. London. Blackwell Scientific Publications.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Schantz P.G., Moritani T., Karlson E., Johansson E., Lundh A. (1989) Maximal voluntary force of bilateral and unilateral leg extension. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 136, 185-192.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Secher N.H., Rorsgaard S., Secher O. (1978) contralateral influence on recruitment of curarized muscle fibres during maximal voluntary extension of the legs. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 103, 456-462.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Secher N.H., Rube N., Elers J. (1988) Strength of two- and one-leg extension in man. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 134, 333-339.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Siebert T., Sust M., Thaller S., Tilp M., Wagner H. (2007) An improved method to determine neuromuscular properties using force laws - From single muscle to applications in human movements. Hum Mov Sci 26, 320-341.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Skelton D.A., Kennedy J., Rutherford O.M. (2002) Explosive power and asymmetry in leg muscle function in frequent fallers and non-fallers aged over 65. Age and Ageing 31, 119-125.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Taniguchi Y. (1998) relationship between the modifications of bilateral deficit in upper and lower limbs by resistance training in humans. European Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology 78, 226-230.
    Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Wagner H., Thaller S., Dahse R., Sust M. (2006) Biomechanical muscle properties and angiotensin-converting enzyme gene polymorphism: a model-based study. Eur J Appl Physiol 98, 507-515.
     
     
     
    Home Issues About Authors
    Contact Current Editorial board Authors instructions
    Email alerts In Press Mission For Reviewers
    Archive Scope
    Supplements Statistics
    Most Read Articles
      Most Cited Articles
     
      
     
    JSSM | Copyright 2001-2024 | All rights reserved. | LEGAL NOTICES | Publisher

    It is forbidden the total or partial reproduction of this web site and the published materials, the treatment of its database, any kind of transition and for any means, either electronic, mechanic or other methods, without the previous written permission of the JSSM.

    This work is licensed under a Creative Commons License Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.